The Function of Morphemes
Word classes
We have seen that morphemes can be bound (e.g. –ly, –ing, –ed) or free (e.g. go, stop, sit). Now, all free morphemes exist within a word class. Each word class incorporates words that all perform a similar function. In English, we recognize ten word classes:
- lexical verbs
- nouns
- adjectives
- adverbs
- auxiliary verbs
- numerals
- determiners
- pronouns
- prepositions
- conjunctions
So, bound and free morphemes combine to create words within the above word classes. However, not all morphemes function in the same way.
Derivation
Class-maintaining
Derivational morphemes create new words from existing words, i.e. new words are derived from their use. They may be either prefixes or suffixes.
Consider how the root morpheme organize can be altered by the addition of the prefixes re– and dis-. The original root (organize) is a verb and the newly derived words re-organize and dis-organize are also verbs. The word class has not altered by the addition of the prefix. Such derivational prefixes are, therefore, said to be class-maintaining.
Class-changing
Now consider what happens when suffixes are appended.
Again recall how the root verb organize may be altered by the addition of –ation to create organiz-ation. This time, the newly derived word is a noun. Similarly, if we append the suffix –er to create the word organiz-er this new word is also a noun. Thus, these suffixes have altered the word class of the word to which they are appended, i.e. they are class-changing. The addition of the suffix –ise will typically create not a noun but a verb from an adjective, e.g. private (adjective) to private-ise (verb), and the addition of –al typically creates an adjective from a noun, e.g. nation (noun) to nation-al (adjective).
Inflexion
Inflexional morphemes always function grammatically in English and, without exception, they are all suffixes. Their function is to alter the grammatical category of a root morpheme.
So, for example, the suffix –s may serve to alter the grammatical category from singular to plural, e.g. dog (singular noun) to dog-s (plural noun). Similarly, the suffix –ed is typically used to change the grammatical category from present tense to past tense, e.g. walk (present tense verb) to walk-ed (past tense verb). Note that it is only the grammatical category that changes with inflexion and not the word class. For example, dog is a noun and if we append the suffix –s the resultant word dog-s is also a noun. Similarly walk is a verb, as is walk-ed. Inflexion is, therefore, class-maintaining.
The emergence of morphemes
A prime motivation when people communicate is to transmit meaning as efficiently as possible.
One way the developing child can reduce ambiguity is by increasing the length of utterances by using more free morphemes (in which case the mean length of utterance will increase). However, it should be apparent that children also reduce ambiguity through an increased use of bound morphemes. For example, if I am attempting to communicate the fact that I have more than one dog then the ability to use the bound morpheme -s to mark a noun as plural reduces potential uncertainty.
Similarly, the ability to use the regular past tense bound morpheme –ed also assists in removing any ambiguity as to whether or not I am talking about current events or events that happened prior to the time of speaking, e.g. I wash the dog as opposed to I washed the dogs.
It would appear that children acquire the use of morphemes in a particular order. This order of acquisition is largely invariant amongst all children learning a particular language. However, the rate at which individual children acquire morphemes will vary considerably. It is thought that they first learn the most frequently occurring morphemes in the language that is spoken to them. For English speaking children the first three morphemes to be acquired are: (1) –ing, as in sing-ing and play-ing, (2) –s, as in dog-s and cat-s, and (3) –ed, as in walk-ed and talk-ed (see Table 3 in Language Delay for a complete outline of the order of acquisition of English morphemes).