SLTinfo logo

Grammar and the Speaker

Meaningful units

If you were asked what the meaningful units of language were called you would probably say ‘words’. Look at the words in the following utterances:

Bill hit Bob

Bob hit Bill

Do the words mean the same thing in each utterance? Do the two utterances mean the same thing? Obviously the answer to the first question is ‘yes’ and the second ‘no’. It follows then that the order in which the words occur has a bearing on the meaning of an utterance. The fact that we can interpret the two utterances correctly shows that we know something about word order which allows us to access that meaning. Knowing a language means more than knowing the words of a language; we need also to know how to put words together in a way which is both predictable and meaningful. The rules which govern how elements of language are put together are known as the grammar of the language. Although this definition appears straightforward the term is used with a range of related but distinct meanings (see Grammatical Description). For example, traditional grammar and prescriptive grammar are principally concerned with supporting a particular (Standard) variety of the language, whereas descriptive grammar tries to view language objectively and to explain all its forms and often the judgements of native speakers too.

Types of grammatical knowledge

There are actually two distinct kinds of grammatical knowledge.

Implicit knowledge

Implicit knowledge enables us, without reflection, to form utterances which our fellow speakers recognise as grammatical (i.e. which conform to the rules of our grammar). It also allows us to identify utterances which are not grammatical.

Explicit knowledge

In addition to our implicit knowledge, we may also have explicit knowledge. If so, then when we hear an ungrammatical utterance we are able to describe the ‘errors’ and state which rules have been broken, probably with the use of technical terminology.

The first kind of knowledge (implicit) is acquired in the natural process of learning to speak; the second kind (explicit) implies explicit teaching.

Activity 1: Using your implicit language knowledge

Which of the following utterances do you consider to be ‘correct’? Explain your reasons; how much grammatical terminology do you need to use?

  1. John and I went to the shops
  2. me and John went to the shops
  3. he came back with sweets for Helen and me
  4. he came back with sweets for Helen and I

Reflection

1 and 3 are the ‘correct’ Standard English. If you could state that much then you have an implicit knowledge of (at least this part of) Standard English.

You may be able to explain your judgement by checking what the utterance sounds like without the ‘extra’ word.

  1. I went to the shops
  2. *me went to the shops
  3. he came back with sweets for me
  4. *he came back with sweets for I

NB: Conventionally, ungrammatical items are marked with a star (asterisk) *.

It is now immediately obvious that the starred utterances are not ‘grammatical’.

Notice that word is a technical term which helps you talk about language (all such terminology is known as metalanguage). You don’t need to know the word word in order to speak and your use of it would be evidence of some explicit language knowledge. If you knew that the extra word was called a noun or that pronouns functioning as objects then your explicit knowledge is all the greater.

Grammar as psychological structures

Many definitions of the term ‘grammar’ refer to written descriptions of language patterns. However, the term can also be applied to those structures within the mind of a speaker which allow the formation and recognition of acceptable utterances. Some forms of descriptive grammar have the description of these psychological rules as their goal and the term ‘grammar’ is often used ambiguously to refer to both the rules in the speaker’s mind and the same rules written down. There is plenty of evidence that these psychological grammars are real; how else can the consistency of language forms be explained? It also seems to be the case that our personal grammars are far more complex than any ‘explicit’ grammar yet written. Every speaker has to learn the forms of their native language anew and one consequence of this is that the grammars of no two speakers will be exactly alike. We each have a personal grammar.

Activity 2: Using your implicit language knowledge

Which of these utterances do you consider to be ‘correct’? Can you explain why you find certain utterances unacceptable? Are there any you are uncertain about? Can you explain why? Are there any which you might say yourself, even if you consider them to be incorrect? Which kind of grammatical knowledge are you applying when you judge these utterances?

  1. My aunt has measles.
  2. Eat this with a left-handed bus.
  3. If I’d have known that he’d still be alive.
  4. This is the world we live in.
  5. From whom is that letter?
  6. That’s the man who I saw yesterday.
  7. I didn’t do nothing.
  8. He climbed out of the window.
  9. My aunt bucket measles.
  10. He got off of the bus.
  11. Who is that letter from?

When you have done this activity for yourself, ask a friend to do it and share your conclusions – do you agree in every case?

Reflection

The list includes some utterances which most speakers of English would be happy with. One utterance is definitely grammatical but makes no sense (2), while one clearly does not follow the basic rules of utterance structure (9). At least one follows a pattern used in many non-Standard varieties and so is grammatical within that variety (7). Others you might feel are condemned by prescriptive grammar even though you feel comfortable saying them (6?, 10?, 11?). Remember in such cases that usage changes over time and that even if you remember rules about ‘who’ and ‘whom’ and about prepositions at the end of utterances, the fact that almost everybody says Who is it from? is an important consideration.

You probably found that if you did this activity with a friend, even if you feel you speak the same variety of English, you did not agree on every point. This illustrates the fact that we all develop our own individual form of English (and explains why so many disputes arise over ‘correctness’.

Summary

All speakers possess their own grammar and employ grammatical rules automatically.

For every utterance which you know implicitly to be grammatical there is an underlying linguistic structure which is susceptible to analysis and description.

Acknowledgements

[Information last accessed: 27 July 2017]

This article is adapted from ‘Grammar: the rules of language’ in Primary English by Ian Eyres which is taken from ‘Knowledge in everyday life’. An OpenLearn (http://www.open.edu/openlearn/) chunk reworked by permission of The Open University copyright © 2016 – made available under the terms of the Creative Commons Licence v4.0 http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-sa/4.0/deed.en_GB. As such, it is also made available under the same licence agreement.