ELISION
Elision explained
Another process that arises as a consequence of rapidly articulated speech and which also operates at word boundaries is elision. Elision is the removal or deletion of a sound, or sounds[1]. It can occur across word boundaries in connected speech.
Word-initial /h/
A frequent elision is the deletion of /h/ when it appears in word-initial position after a word boundary, e.g.
she lives in Hull | /ʃi lɪvz ɪn hʌl/ | → | [ʃɪ lɪvz ɪ̃n ʌ̃ɫ] |
he’s very happy | /hiz vɛrɪ hæpɪ / | → | [(h)iːz vɛɹɪ æpʰɪ] |
The elision of word-initial /h/ is not especially predictable. However, unstressed pronouns beginning with /h/ appear to be susceptible, i.e. he, him, her, his, hers, himself, herself. Here are three examples:
he went there | /hi wɛnt ðɛə/ | → | [ɪ wɛn̪t̪̚ ðɛəː] |
give him it | /gɪv hɪm ɪt/ | → | [g̟ɪv ɪm ɪʔ] |
give her it | /gɪv hɜ ðɛm/ | → | [g̟ɪv ɜː ðəm] |
In addition, forms of the auxiliary verb have also appear to be susceptible to so-called h-dropping, e.g.
would have | /wʊd hæv/ | → | [ɪ wʊd əv̥] |
could have | /kʊd hæv/ | → | [k̠ʰʊd əv̥] |
might have | /maɪt hæv / | → | [mãɪt əv̥] |
In addition to these features that frequently appear in rapidly articulated standard British English, h-dropping is also a feature of several accents, including Yorkshire, Cumbrian and Cockney. We can summarize this simply as follows.
Word-initial /h/ is frequently omitted.
Word-final clusters
The alveolar plosives /t/ and /d/ are particularly susceptible to elision when they appear in word-final position if they are (1) preceded by a consonant and, (2) followed by a consonant which is in word-initial position of the immediately following word. This can be summarized as follows.
Predominantly, the consonants that combine with /t/ to create two-member clusters in word-final position are voiceless consonants, i.e. the voicing of the preceding consonant and the following /t/ is voiceless. In contrast, the consonants that combine with /d/ to create two-member clusters in word-final position are voiced consonants, i.e. the voicing of the preceding consonant and the following /d/ is voiced. This gives rise to many possible clusters (Table 1).
Table 1. Matrix of two-member word-final clusters with /t/ and /d/.
Some two-member clusters do not appear in any English words in word-final position. These are the clusters presented in Table 1 with strikeout, i.e. /tt, θt, ht, dd, wd, rd, jd/. The remaining clusters can be found with different frequencies in English words. The following examples show how a ‘voiceless consonant plus /t/’ cluster in word-final position elides the /t/ when the cluster appears before another consonant.
cluster | example | |||
/-pt/ | kept quiet | /kɛpt kwaɪət/ | → | [k̟ʰɛp̚ kʰwaɪːəʔ] |
/-kt/ | licked two | /lɪkt tu/ | → | [lɪk̚ tuː] |
/-ft/ | left luggage | /lɛft lʌgɪʤ/ | → | [lɛf lʌg̠ɪʤ̊] |
/-st/ | last man | /lɑst mæn/ | → | [lɑs mæ̃n] |
/-ʃt/ | mashed potato | /mæʃt pəteɪtəʊ/ | → | [mæ̃ʃ pʰətʰeɪtʰəʊ] |
/-ʧt/ | beached whale | /biʧt weɪl/ | → | [biʧ weɪːɫ] |
The following demonstrate how a ‘voiced consonant plus /d/’ cluster in word-final position elides the /d/ when the cluster appears before another consonant.
cluster | example | |||
/-bd/ | mobbed team | /mɒbd tim/ | → | [mɒ̃b̚ tʰĩːm] |
/-gd/ | lagged tank | /lægd tæŋk/ | → | [læg̚ tʰæ̃ŋkʰ] |
/-md/ | roamed far | /rəʊmd fɑ/ | → | [ɹəʊ̃ːm fɑː] |
/-nd/ | hand cart | /hænd kɑt/ | → | [hæ̃ŋ k̠ʰɑːʔ] |
/-ŋd/ | winged bird | /wɪŋd bɜd/ | → | [wɪ̃ŋ bɜːd̥] |
/-vd/ | loved dog | /lʌvd bɔɪ/ | → | [lʌv bɔɪː] |
/-ðd/ | bathed quickly | /beɪðd kwɪklɪ/ | → | [beɪːð kʰwɪkʰlɪ] |
/-zd/ | used car | /juzd kɑ/ | → | [juːz̥ k̠ʰɑː] |
/-ʒd/ | garaged car | /gærɑʒd kɑ/ | → | [g̟æɹɑːʒ̊ k̠ʰɑː] |
/-ʤd/ | changed places | /ʧeɪnʤd pleɪsɪz/ | → | [ʧeɪ̃ːnʤ̊ pʰl̥eɪsɪz̥] |
/-ld/ | bold font | /bəʊld fɒnt/ | → | [bəʊːɫ fɒ̃nʔ] |
/nt/ clusters
The consonant cluster /nt/ differs from those discussed above in that the voicing of the two members is not matched. In an /nt/cluster the first consonant is voiced whilst the second consonant is voiceless. It can occur in words such as haven’t /hævnt/ and hint /hɪnt/. The /t/ in these clusters can also be omitted when they occur before a word-initial consonant across a word boundary, e.g.
cluster | example | |||
/-nt/ | she didn’t do it | /ʃi dɪdnt du ɪt/ | → | [ʃi dɪdn duː ɪʔ] |
a hint of mint | /eɪ hɪnt ɒv lɛmən/ | → | [ə hɪ̃n əv lɛ̃mə̃n] |
The elision of /t/ and /d/ in word-final clusters, as discussed in this subsection, can be summarized as follows.
Word-final /t, d/ preceded by a consonant and followed by a consonant in word-initial position are omitted.
The many examples of this process presented here demonstrate the pervasiveness of elision in connected speech. These examples, together with the numerous examples of de-alveolar assimilations, convincingly show that alveolars are especially susceptible to alteration in connected speech.
Notes
[1] An elision of a single consonant within a word is known as consonant deletion, e.g. dog /dɒɡ/ → /dɒ/; back /bæk/ → /æk/. An elision of more than one consonant in a sequence is known as cluster reduction, e.g. flower /flaʊə/ → /faʊə/; last /lɑst/ → /lɑs/. These are examples of so-called phonological simplifying processes.
NEXT>> REDUCTION