SLTinfo logo

ELISION

go to contents icon

Elision explained

Another process that arises as a consequence of rapidly articulated speech and which also operates at word boundaries is elision. Elision is the removal or deletion of a sound, or sounds[1]. It can occur across word boundaries in connected speech.

Word-initial /h/

A frequent elision is the deletion of /h/ when it appears in word-initial position after a word boundary, e.g.

she lives in Hull /ʃi lɪvz ɪn hʌl/ [ʃɪ lɪvz ɪ̃n ʌ̃ɫ]
he’s very happy /hiz vɛrɪ hæpɪ / [(h)iːz vɛɹɪ æpʰɪ]

The elision of word-initial /h/ is not especially predictable. However, unstressed pronouns beginning with /h/ appear to be susceptible, i.e. he, him, her, his, hers, himself, herself. Here are three examples:

he went there /hi wɛnt ðɛə/ [ɪ wɛn̪t̪̚ ðɛəː]
give him it /gɪv hɪm ɪt/ [g̟ɪv ɪm ɪʔ]
give her it /gɪv hɜ ðɛm/ [g̟ɪv ɜː ðəm]

In addition, forms of the auxiliary verb have also appear to be susceptible to so-called h-dropping, e.g.

would have /wʊd hæv/ [ɪ wʊd əv̥]
could have /kʊd hæv/ [k̠ʰʊd əv̥]
might have /maɪt hæv / [mãɪt əv̥]

In addition to these features that frequently appear in rapidly articulated standard British English, h-dropping is also a feature of several accents, including Yorkshire, Cumbrian and Cockney. We can summarize this simply as follows.

Word-initial /h/ is frequently omitted.

Word-final clusters

The alveolar plosives /t/ and /d/ are particularly susceptible to elision when they appear in word-final position if they are (1) preceded by a consonant and, (2) followed by a consonant which is in word-initial position of the immediately following word. This can be summarized as follows.

elision process 1

Predominantly, the consonants that combine with /t/ to create two-member clusters in word-final position are voiceless consonants, i.e. the voicing of the preceding consonant and the following /t/ is voiceless. In contrast, the consonants that combine with /d/ to create two-member clusters in word-final position are voiced consonants, i.e. the voicing of the preceding consonant and the following /d/ is voiced. This gives rise to many possible clusters (Table 1).

Table 1. Matrix of two-member word-final clusters

Table 1. Matrix of two-member word-final clusters with /t/ and /d/.

Some two-member clusters do not appear in any English words in word-final position. These are the clusters presented in Table 1 with strikeout, i.e. /tt, θt, ht, dd, wd, rd, jd/. The remaining clusters can be found with different frequencies in English words. The following examples show how a ‘voiceless consonant plus /t/’ cluster in word-final position elides the /t/ when the cluster appears before another consonant.

cluster example
/-pt/ kept quiet /kɛpt kwaɪət/ [k̟ʰɛp̚ kʰwaɪːəʔ]
/-kt/ licked two /lɪkt tu/ [lɪk̚ tuː]
/-ft/ left luggage /lɛft lʌgɪʤ/ [lɛf lʌg̠ɪʤ̊]
/-st/ last man /lɑst mæn/ [lɑs mæ̃n]
/-ʃt/ mashed potato /mæʃt pəteɪtəʊ/ [mæ̃ʃ pʰətʰeɪtʰəʊ]
/-ʧt/ beached whale /biʧt weɪl/ [biʧ weɪːɫ]

The following demonstrate how a ‘voiced consonant plus /d/’ cluster in word-final position elides the /d/ when the cluster appears before another consonant.

cluster example
/-bd/ mobbed team /mɒbd tim/ [mɒ̃b̚ tʰĩːm]
/-gd/ lagged tank /lægd tæŋk/ [læg̚ tʰæ̃ŋkʰ]
/-md/ roamed far /rəʊmd fɑ/ [ɹəʊ̃ːm fɑː]
/-nd/ hand cart /hænd kɑt/ [hæ̃ŋ k̠ʰɑːʔ]
/-ŋd/ winged bird /wɪŋd bɜd/ [wɪ̃ŋ bɜːd̥]
/-vd/ loved dog /lʌvd bɔɪ/ [lʌv bɔɪː]
/-ðd/ bathed quickly /beɪðd kwɪklɪ/ [beɪːð kʰwɪkʰlɪ]
/-zd/ used car /juzd kɑ/ [juːz̥ k̠ʰɑː]
/-ʒd/ garaged car /gærɑʒd kɑ/ [g̟æɹɑːʒ̊ k̠ʰɑː]
/-ʤd/ changed places /ʧeɪnʤd pleɪsɪz/ [ʧeɪ̃ːnʤ̊ pʰl̥eɪsɪz̥]
/-ld/ bold font /bəʊld fɒnt/ [bəʊːɫ fɒ̃nʔ]

/nt/ clusters

The consonant cluster /nt/ differs from those discussed above in that the voicing of the two members is not matched. In an /nt/cluster the first consonant is voiced whilst the second consonant is voiceless. It can occur in words such as haven’t /hævnt/ and hint /hɪnt/. The /t/ in these clusters can also be omitted when they occur before a word-initial consonant across a word boundary, e.g.

cluster example
/-nt/ she didn’t do it /ʃi dɪdnt du ɪt/ [ʃi dɪdn duː ɪʔ]
a hint of mint /eɪ hɪnt ɒv lɛmən/ [ə hɪ̃n əv lɛ̃mə̃n]

The elision of /t/ and /d/ in word-final clusters, as discussed in this subsection, can be summarized as follows.

Word-final /t, d/ preceded by a consonant and followed by a consonant in word-initial position are omitted.

The many examples of this process presented here demonstrate the pervasiveness of elision in connected speech. These examples, together with the numerous examples of de-alveolar assimilations, convincingly show that alveolars are especially susceptible to alteration in connected speech.

Notes

[1] An elision of a single consonant within a word is known as consonant deletion, e.g. dog /dɒɡ/ → /dɒ/; back /bæk/ → /æk/. An elision of more than one consonant in a sequence is known as cluster reduction, e.g. flower /flaʊə/ → /faʊə/; last /lɑst/ → /lɑs/. These are examples of so-called phonological simplifying processes.

NEXT>> REDUCTION