De-voicing
De-voicing of plosives
Consider how the voiced bilabial plosive /b/ is articulated in the word ball /bɔl/. You will probably need to say it aloud to both hear and feel the effect. Now try saying aloud the word nib /nɪb/, again focusing on the phoneme /b/. Do they sound the same? For most speakers, the /b/ at the beginning of the word ball /bɔl/ is fully voiced – it is a strong sound. However, the /b/ at the end of the word nib /nɪb/ is typically less forceful – it is not fully voiced. This phonetic realization is referred to as being de-voiced and this de-voiced allophone of /b/ is represented symbolically as [b̥]. We can say, therefore, that /b/ has at least two allophones, [b] and [b̥].
Of course, only voiced consonants can be de-voiced and the remaining two voiced plosives /d, g/ are similarly de-voiced in word-final position, e.g.
bed | /bɛd/ | → | [bɛd̥] | |
bag | /bæg/ | → | [bæɡ̊] |
De-voicing of fricatives
The voiced fricatives /v/, /ð/ and /z/ are also similarly voiced in word-final position, e.g.
have | /hæv/ | → | [hæv̥] | |
seethe | /sið/ | → | [siːð̥] | |
haze | /heɪz/ | → | [heɪːz̥] |
As we have seen, the voiced post-alveolar fricative /ʒ/ typically occurs intervocalically in English. Exceptionally, when it does occur word-finally in loan words it may also be subject to de-voicing, e.g.
beige | /beɪʒ/ | → | [beɪːʒ̊] | |
garage | / gærɒʒ/ | → | [gærɒːʒ̊] |
There are, however, only a handful of words where /ʒ/ occurs in word-final position and so the frequency of occurrence for de-voicing of the post-alveolar fricative is extremely low.
De-voicing of affricates
As we know, there is only one voiced affricate in English, the voiced post-alveolar affricate /ʤ/. As with plosives and fricatives, this affricate is also de-voiced in word-final position, e.g.
badge | /bæʤ/ | → | [bæʤ̊] |
De-voicing of approximants
Of the four approximants, only the liquids /r/ and /l/ show any appreciable de-voicing in particular contexts. There is a de-voiced allophone of the phoneme /r/ which occurs after the voiceless consonants /p, t, k, f, θ, ʃ/. It is represented symbolically as [ɹ̥] and occurs in words such as the following.
pray | /preɪ/ | → | [pʰɹ̥eɪː] | |
tray | /treɪ/ | → | [tʰr̥eɪː] | |
cry | /kraɪ/ | → | [kʰɹ̥aɪː] | |
free | /fri/ | → | [fɹ̥iː] | |
three | /θri/ | → | [θɹ̥iː] | |
shrew | /ʃru/ | → | [ʃɹ̥uː] |
The fully voiced allophone is represented symbolically as [ɹ] and this occurs elsewhere, e.g.
run | /rʌn/ | → | [ɹʌn] | |
carry | /kærɪ/ | → | [kʰæɹɪ] |
Thus, the phoneme /r/ has at least two allophones: [ɹ] and [ɹ̥].
The lateral /l/ is de-voiced when it immediately follows either the voiceless bilabial plosive /p/ or the voiceless velar plosive /k/. For example:
play | /pleɪ/ | → | [pʰl̥eɪː] | |
clay | /kleɪ/ | → | [kʰl̥eɪː] |
The fully voiced allophone [l], however, occurs before vowels, e.g.
lay | /leɪ/ | → | [leɪː] | |
early | /ɜlɪ/ | → | [ɜːlɪ] |
Consequently, the phoneme /l/ has at least two allophones: [l] and [l̥].
Table 6. De-voiced allophones.
NEXT>> Velarization